Merkabah | Page 440 | INFJ Forum
What? I didn't? I actually had a really bad one.
That's why I mentioned the egg sambo. I don't want people to go through my pain.

Well...I guess it was all for naught then.
You seemed awfully pleased at the time with the after-effects and result.
Shush it you.

:m025:
 
Good stuff here!
Thoughts?

Enjoy!



JOHN CLEESE TALKS TO DR JIM TUCKER ABOUT
RESEARCH INTO CHILDREN’S PAST LIFE MEMORIES


5760.jpg

Regular readers will know that legendary comedian and Monty Python alumni John Cleese has a deep interest in research into the survival of consciousness beyond death.

And if you’re interested in that topic, the place to go is the Division of Perceptual Studies (DoPS) at the University of Virginia, which has hosted researchers of the calibre of Dr Bruce Greyson (NDEs) and the late Dr Ian Stevenson (reincarnation memories).

So it’s no surprise to see a video posted recently online (and embedded below) by the DoPS in which another researcher there, Dr Jim Tucker, is interviewed by John Cleese himself.

In the nine-minute-long video, Tucker gives a short history of the reincarnation research performed by the DoPS since the 1960s, beginning with Ian Stevenson, through which they have now collected 2500 cases of past-life recollection.

He then goes into detail about how they collect case information and evidence, along with a description of one of their ‘best’ evidential cases.





John Cleese is a friend and supporter of the research being done at UVA DOPS into the nature of consciousness.
Mr. Cleese has a long history of personal interest in this important area of inquiry.

In this video John Cleese is interviewing pediatric psychiatrist and director of UVA DOPS,
Dr. Jim Tucker, about his research into the memories of young children who appear to recall specific factual memories of a previous life.

In this video, Dr. Tucker presents some of the details of the case of
‘Ryan’ who recalled specific facts about the life of a man name ‘Marty Martin’.

The Division of Perceptual Studies (DOPS) is a research unit within the
Department of Psychiatry and Neurobehavioral Sciences at the University of Virginia Health System.

The research faculty of the Division are known internationally for their expertise and research integrity
in the investigation of phenomena relevant to the nature of consciousness and its relationship to the physical world.

You can learn more about the Division of Perceptual Studies at their website.​
 
Well...I guess it was all for naught then.
You seemed awfully pleased at the time with the after-effects and result.
Shush it you.

:m025:

Don't try to invalidate my pain.
Maybe if you had offered the egg sandwich I would have reached new heights of spiritual enlightenment.
It's the least you could have done. Y'know apart from buying the fun guys, being a guide, watching out for me, and patiently giving me your input on my experience.
I think your apathy and selfishness here has ruptured our friendship forever.
:m206:
 
Don't try to invalidate my pain.
Maybe if you had offered the egg sandwich I would have reached new heights of spiritual enlightenment.
It's the least you could have done. Y'know apart from buying the fun guys, being a guide, watching out for me, and patiently giving me your input on my experience.
I think your apathy and selfishness here has ruptured our friendship forever.
:m206:

3f4.gif
 
Always thought provoking to listen to.
Enjoy!

Dr. Gabor Maté
Toxic Culture



The Canadian physician and best-selling author of In the Realm of Hungry Ghosts
is a brilliantly original thinker on addiction, trauma, parenting and the social context of human diseases and imbalances.

Contrary to the assumptions of mainstream medicine, he asserts that most human ailments are not individual problems,
but reflections of a person's relationship with the physical, emotional and social environment, from conception to death.

Mind and body are not separate in real life,
and thus health and illness in a person reflect social and economic realities more than personal predispositions.

In other words, personal responsibility cannot be separated from societal responsibility and changing the world.



 
@Skarekrow he is just awesome, the way he delivers and his presence..


This one is pure gold

Thanks!
Yes, he’s one of my favorite speakers and definitely knows what he is talking about.
I am always impressed with what he has to say...especially regarding the addicted mind.
One particular stat he was talking about in another speech always stuck with me...this is paraphrased but it was basically “I’ve never met a female heroin user that has not been abused in some manner growing up.”
Many attempt to self-medicate with drugs to cover up the abuse...to then only be punished again by the criminal justice system.
In essence we are punishing people multiple times for the crime of being broken.

Nice to make your acquaintance...please enjoy the thread and let me know if I can help you find any information along the lines of this subjects here.
Thanks for the video again!
 
Thanks!
Yes, he’s one of my favorite speakers and definitely knows what he is talking about.
I am always impressed with what he has to say...especially regarding the addicted mind.
One particular stat he was talking about in another speech always stuck with me...this is paraphrased but it was basically “I’ve never met a female heroin user that has not been abused in some manner growing up.”
Many attempt to self-medicate with drugs to cover up the abuse...to then only be punished again by the criminal justice system.
In essence we are punishing people multiple times for the crime of being broken.

Nice to make your acquaintance...please enjoy the thread and let me know if I can help you find any information along the lines of this subjects here.
Thanks for the video again!

Yes, that's why i like him, because he knows how addicted mind works. I was addict too and that's what got my attention. He is also very compassionate and kind..
Thank you for the nice words :)
 
Yes, that's why i like him, because he knows how addicted mind works. I was addict too and that's what got my attention. He is also very compassionate and kind..
Thank you for the nice words :)

It’s amazing and also very sad that there is such a negative stigma against those who are “addicts” and it’s even sillier when we consider what is a socially acceptable drug and what isn’t...such as alcohol.
Alcohol is just as dangerous, if not more dangerous as the majority of drugs out there, including drugs that are classified as “illegal”, both in harm to the self and harm to others.
Drunk drivers kill people, but very often survive because they are too drunk to react and break bones by tensing up.
Meanwhile....my nearly 70 year old Mom had to jump through flaming hoops just to get a very small prescription for pain meds when she has a plethora of legitimate issues needing assistance.
Other doctors cut off some of their pain patients and left them high and dry...where do you think they turn?
To illegal sources of course.
This is where you find people overdosing...this is where you find carfentanyl cut in killing people.
Even the medications we have here to help those get off such addictive meds are usually not covered by many insurance policies.
It’s idiotic.
It’s far easier to blame those who are the most vulnerable...and granted, many of them turn to illegal and unsavory means to get their next fix - but that should really tell people how incredibly powerful some of these addictions can be.
At one point I was legitimately prescribed fentanyl, methadone, and diluadid (hydromorphone), together all at moderate doses and I became so hyperanagesic (in more pain) that I had to just stop taking them altogether.
It was a very painful process to detox as I choose to go cold turkey.
It’s not only mentally painful, but it is literally physically painful and emotionally you feel such anxiety I didn’t sleep the first 4 days at least.
Anyhow.
There is no stigma concerning such discussions around me - I reserve judgement.
That was the first time I had to detox, lol.
All I was taking was legitimately prescribed and for legit reasons ( I have ankylosing spondylitis) ...but if I couldn’t get help, I am not sure where I might have turned in the past?
Now I am much more open about my ideas regarding certain medications and whatnot and some have outright changed.
For instance...I refuse to take fentanyl or methadone ever again, but have found great relief in psilocybin from mushrooms on the rare occasion used therapeutically.
Much more so than anything ever prescribed to me over the course of my life.

It’s a tricky subject.
 



60483715_668368750278071_3815433420012519424_n.jpg

59745456_2442210732478221_7759559534980366336_n.jpg

59782751_2435365203162774_2884230376471920640_n.jpg



60467198_2454095214623106_8893216118399827968_n.jpg

60580059_2455117967854164_2116943003141537792_n.jpg

61139269_10211464093198025_9071246077641883648_n.jpg


61215206_2391015704295971_724397189962399744_n.jpg


Terence-McKenna.jpg


 
Last edited:
Very curious....
The hell if I will ever create a Tulpa on purpose though.
(Pretty sure I have on accident once or twice)
Tulpas originally did NOT just live in a persons’ head (as part of the article implies), but rather could act independently and would sometimes even turn on the creator...people have been possessed, physically harmed, etc.
This article does not consider any spiritual aspects of this phenomena which makes it incomplete imho as this is the history and nature of a Tulpa...not an online forum for imaginary friend enthusiasts, lol.
This is a totally materialist scientific viewpoint of the phantasm being created by the mind.
But I find it is good to not just feed my own confirmation bias and keep a well rounded understanding.
No matter what this dude writes about Tulpas though...I will never fuck around “creating” one.

Enjoy!​


Daring to Hear Voices
Why Tulpamancers have a lot to teach us about what we can imagine and experience
https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/culture-mind-and-brain/201604/daring-hear-voices

Screen%20Shot%202016-04-15%20at%2011.34.32%20AM.png

Source: "The Intention to Know" / Theosophical Society​

Making the strange familiar

The new subculture of Tulpamancy has garnered a lot of online attention of late.
Tulpas, a concept borrowed from Tibetan Buddhism, are sentient imaginary friends conjured through ‘thoughtform’ visualization.

Tulpamancers are people who conjure Tulpas, and experience their imaginary companions as semi-permanent, non-threatening auditory ‘hallucinations’.

Other sense modalities like touch, emotions, and vision are also recruited in the experience.

Tulpamancers have been called the weirdest culture on the internet.
As a cultural phenomenon, the practice has been described as a strange secularization of the paranormal.

In the blogosphere, people have wondered if Tulpamancers have underlying mental illnesses, and if it is possible to hear voices without being crazy. Others have wondered if they are telling the truth.

How is it possible—is it possible at all?—to create a mental being that lives inside your head?

In this article, the first of two posts on the subject, I address popular myths and questions about Tulpamancy, and show that there is nothing inherently strange about the practice.

I expand on its positive and therapeutic aspects, and argue that studying Tulpamancy can help us move beyond simplistic understandings of mental illness.

I also present this new phenomenon as a fascinating example to understand the influence of culture on inner experiences.
In doing so, I invite readers to consider the limitations that contemporary culture places on imagination, our senses, and what we accept as real, normal, and desirable.

As a cognitive anthropologist who has studied Tulpamancy closely, I have sought to apply the old intellectual recipe of making the strange familiar and making the familiar strange.

This is an approach that was championed by Margaret Mead, a key early figure in my discipline.
In her study of growing up in Samoa in the 1920s, Mead examined the ‘strange’ culture of adolescents in the Western Pacific who did not appear to undergo the ‘normal’ stress and turmoil of what was then (as it still is now) understood to be a difficult hormonally-mediated transition from childhood into adulthood.

The lack of sexual restrictions in ‘adolescent’ lives in Samoa at the time had also seemed strange to Mead.
Returning to the U.S., she now saw what she had taken for granted with fresh eyes.

Could it be, she asked, that the distress experienced by American teenagers and the taboos on youth sexuality in western culture at large were in fact quite strange?

Could it be that what she had assumed to be a universally harrowing human experience was in fact grounded in the specific ways of a particular culture at a particular time?

Why Tulpamancers are not crazy.

In seeking to make the strange familiar, I have found that Tulpamancers, far from being crazy, were simply cultivating fundamentally normal dimensions of human cognition and sociality.

I describe these mechanisms in part 2 of this series.

Tulpamancers reported overwhelmingly positive experiences, overall increased happiness, and more confidence in challenging social situations through the assistance of their Tulpa companions.

Many of those who had identified with specific psychopathological labels like depression, anxiety, or ADHD similarly spoke of overall improvement.
When queried independently, Tulpas often described being ‘immune’ to the specific conditions of their hosts.

Autism Spectrum Disorders presented something of an exception.
One Tulpa explained that “having the same brain” as his host, both were necessarily bound to similar limitations.

Others reported greater degrees of freedom from their hosts’ conditions..

One of the first conclusions of my research was that conjuring Tulpas could make one more empathetic.
This is not a surprising finding.

Focusing one’s attention and affect on other people (real or imaginary), as we do when we read fiction or watch movies, has been amply demonstrated to increase empathy—that is, to makes us better at intuitively relating to other people, or being able to imagine what it is like to be somebody else in different situations.

Other findings pointed to further therapeutic possibilities.
A small minority of Tulpamancers, for example, already experienced voices before thinking of them as Tulpas, or turning them into friendly companions.

Some simply thought of them as imaginary friends.
Others had had difficult, or scary experiences with their voices and the characters that lived in their mind, and had come to understand them as a sign of illness.

In these instances, simply getting to know the voices, learning to talk to them as friends, and sharing the experience with other Tulpamaners seemed to lead to very positive results.

This approach, once again, is not new.
The dutch psychiatrist Marius Romme, for example, has developed a successful approach called “living with voices” to help people with psychosis turn their voices into friendly ones.

I want to insist that ‘hallucinations’ and ‘psychosis’ are not a productive terms to think of Tulpa experiences, and voice-hearing experiences at large.
It is both too simplistic and inaccurate to think of voice-hearing as a necessarily pathological experience.

In modern-day psychiatry, the presence of non self-authored thoughts is often, but not always understood as a sign of mental illness.
In practice, one can only speak of pathology when there are clear signs of distress.

If a person describes her inner experiences as being scary, stressful, or as preventing her from functioning well in everyday life, or if others around her report being scared or prevented from functioning by her behaviour, then we can safely speak of pathology.

As we have seen, this is far from being the case with the vast majority of Tulpamancers.

Whether ‘thinking’ is always or ever ‘self-authored’ is too complex a philosophical question to tackle here.
It raises, for one, intractable questions about the nature of Consciousness and the Self, and equally hard questions about the problem of Free Will.

It poses very difficult questions about the nature and role of the body, emotions, moods, and drives.
It also raises hard questions about the nature and role of language and culture, their relationship with behaviour, intuition, and inner-narration, and their variations across social groups.

Tulpamancers, as we will see, show us something fascinating about cultural variations in the positivity and negativity of the voice-hearing experience, and the fuzziness of narrative consciousness in general.

But first, we should appreciate what little we know about what goes on inside people’s heads.

Studying inner experience

Many people want to know if Tulpamancers are telling the truth about the experiences.
Their claims do seem hard to validate.

At face value, however, they are no more or less difficult to study than the claims made by anyone about what goes on in their heads.
While we have every reason to believe that people around us are conscious, have inner experiences, feel pleasure and pain, and have streams of narrative in their head, we have absolutely no way to study these experiences scientifically or to prove that any of this is going on.

In philosophy, this is known as the Problem of Other Minds.

For a while, progress in neuroimaging seemed to carry the promise that the so-called Hard Problem of Consciousness would be solved, and that neural underpinnings, or even causes of these processes would be discovered.

But no such breakthrough occurred.
While we can sometimes make good hypotheses about the brain regions associated with different kinds of tasks and behaviours (including thinking about other people) these posited neural “signatures” or “correlates” tell us nothing whatsoever about the content and quality of people’s experiences.

We know, to present an analogy, that people’s heart-rates accelerate when they experience positive (eustress) and negative (stress) arousal.
This is a physiological signature of arousal.

But heart-rate measurements tell us nothing about what the person is feeling.
So it goes with brain imaging.

Verbal reports elicited from individuals or personal introspection, as anecdotal as they seem, are still the best “evidence” we have for any kind of mental and bodily phenomena.

Most people, to make the matter worse, are quite unskilled at noticing, attending to, monitoring, and reporting on minutiae of their experiences, which makes the problem even harder to study.

Working with Tulpamancers, however, like working with meditators, is a treat for phenomenologists (scholars who study inner experience), because they have trained themselves to be more attentive to their experiences than the average population.

When a large group of people report similarly fine-grained experiences that are comparable (in that they differ from average experiences reported by other groups) this is pretty good “evidence” for their veracity.

Relying on first-person reports does not preclude the possibility of quantitative measures.
When I surveyed a group of 160+ Tulpamancers on the quality of their voice-hearing experience, for example, I found that most subjects who reported hearing their Tulpas’ voices “as distinctly as another person’s voice” had been practicing Tulpamancy for two years or more.

Practitioners with less experience, in turn, tended to report voices that were more thought-like, or halfway between their own thoughts and somebody else’s voice.

That tulpamancers in similar stages of the practice describe comparably similar experiences tending toward full-fledged automatized voices adds further validity to these reports.

These findings are consistent with what we are beginning to understand about auditory ‘hallucinations’.
A recent study published in the Lancet, for example, found that against simplistic folk notions of auditory hallucinations heard as actual voices, schizophrenic patients also reported fine-grained distinctions between thought-like and other-voice-like experiences.

Voice-hearing across cultures

Recent work in psychological anthropology has also yielded more myth-busting findings on the content and affective dimensions of voice-hearing across cultures, and the relationship between people, their voices, and the implicit expectations shared by other members of their societies.

In a recent project, Stanford University’s Tanya Luhrmann led a global team of anthropologists and psychiatrists who asked people diagnosed with schizophrenia in India, Ghana, and the United States what their voices were telling them.

Their fascinating results, in classical Margaret Mead fashion, showed that the mean, terrifying, threatening, debilitating character most of us associate with psychosis were much more pronounced with Western patients, and were likely to be rooted in biases inherent to Euro-American culture.

In Ghana and India, patients were more likely to report friendly and guiding voices, and hearing the voices of relatives.
When voices were teasing or mocking, they did so in much less violent ways.

In the Chennai sample, even voices that were actively disliked by patients tended to give commands consistent with family obligations such as “go to the kitchen and prepare food”, or “you need to eat, but not too much”.

In the California sample, patients were much more likely to describe their voices as violent, and spoke of their experience as meaning that they were “crazy”.​

Screen%20Shot%202016-04-15%20at%208.58.45%20AM.png

Source: Tanya M. Luhrmann, Padmavati, Hema Tharoor, Akwasi Osei / Topics in Cognitive Science 7 (2015) 646–663, p650​

Cultural Invitations - Cognitive Ideology

This led Luhrmann and colleagues to develop a theory of “social kindling”, or “cultural invitations” in the mediation of psychosis.
What we pay attention to and how we make sense of it, the authors of the study argued, is always subtly influenced by our culture—that is, by how we expect others around us to think the world works.

They explained that implicit “cultural invitations” on how one should behave, how to make sense of and value experience, but also on what counts as a mind, a person, a spirit, a normal experience, and a pathological one can have an immense effect on how we feel.

This is something I have called “cognitive ideology”, or the power of culturally-specific ideas and biases on what counts as a mind, what counts as real, and what counts as “normal”, desirable, or undesirable experience in shaping our most intuitive modes of affect and action.

Responding to latent cultural beliefs

As we will see, positive and negative inner experiences, and “anomalous” experiences of all kinds also occur on a spectrum of implicit to explicit responses to deeply held, but often unconscious cultural assumptions.

The work of the late psychologist Nicholas Spanos, who spent a lifetime studying such “strange” experiences as hypnosis, multiple personalities, false memories, UFO abduction reports, and past-life recall, played an important role in our understanding of the relationship between culture and inner experience.

Through his clinical experiments and reviews of these strange cases, Spanos developed a sociocognitive hypothesis to explain how subjective reality responds to largely implicit, but elaborately “rule-governed” collectively held ideas.

He famously pointed out, for example, that reports of UFO abductions from individuals who appear convinced that they underwent the experience typically involve alien technology that is collectively imaginable, but not yet attainable.

The first reports of sighting and abductions in the pre-modern period, thus involved flying ships with sails.
It was possible to imagine the not-yet-attainable technology of flying ships, but not yet collectively imaginable to think of ships with no sails.

In the post-Apollo, post-Star Wars era, on this account, it has become collectively imaginable to think of such currently unattainable technologies as light-speed travel and teleportation.

This points to the importance of appreciating the role of culture in shaping one’s latent ideas, or implicitly held beliefs.
Simply put, these are deeply held expectations about what is true, false, right and wrong that we do not know we hold, but that shape our automatic behaviors.

Most of us are not very reflective about our own biases.
They tend to manifest themselves in our most “personal” tastes, preferences, intuitions, and avoidance or attraction mechanisms.

But these responses are, as Spanos would have put it, rule-governed cultural constructs nonetheless.

Racist and sexist biases are infamous examples of such implicit beliefs picked up from latent cultural ideologies.
They can easily be studied in children through attribution tasks, such as the famous Clark Doll Experiment.

In this experiment, children are asked to express their preference for one of two dolls, depicting a black and white babies.
Worryingly, even black children tend to express preference for the white doll.

How does this happen?

In the past 70 years of research on racial bias, studies have consistently shown that across cultures, children as young as age 4 have already acquired biases about ethnicity and other socially constructed categories of persons that are consistent with the dominant culture of their societies.

In most cases, however, these biases are not consciously held by the children’s caregivers and educators, and they are almost never explicitly taught.
It is as though the biases are literally “picked up” from a fuzzy cultural soup.

How such biases are acquired -- indeed, how broader cultural grammars are acquired -- is still an open question.


Luigi-Prina-Flying-Ships-Milan5.jpg

Source: Flying Ships / (c) Luigi Prina, Milan​

Individual Psychological Traits

I presented the mystery of how a latent architecture of cultural expectations and behaviours is acquired, and emphasized that this process extends to hallucinations, imagination, embodiment, and inner experiences at large.

But we should be cautious in assuming an “anything-goes” formula, where anyone can mentally improvise from a public language to hallucinate, conjure sentient voices, feel like they are abducted by aliens, or have out of body experiences.

Spanos’ work, we should note, has been criticized for overemphasizing the social, and failing to give appropriate consideration to the individual psychological traits of people who are more prone to anomalous experiences than others.

Hypnotizability, proneness to absorption (the ability to become fully immersed in one’s inner imagery) and proneness to dissociation are examples of traits that are known to occur on a spectrum across populations, and are likely be be innate.

Fantasy-proneness, a hypothesized sub-type of absorption, has also been identified (albeit more controversially) in people who report anomalous experiences.

Another standard explanation for anomalous experiences is that they occur as a response to repressed memories and trauma.

In a response to his critics, Spanos tested for trauma and traits variables in a study that divided subjects who reported UFO experiences into non intense (e.g., seeing lights and shapes in the sky) and intense (e.g., seeing and communicating with aliens or missing time) groups.

He found that subjects in both groups did not score higher than average on psychopathology, hypnotizability, and fantasy proneness, but that experiences in the intense group were more often sleep-related (e.g., sleep paralysis).

Subjects in the intense group also reported much stronger beliefs in the existence of aliens and space visitation.

Learning to Hear Voices: Explicit beliefs and the training of absorption.

Spanos’ findings on the intense UFO experiences group adds further evidence to the claim that culture shapes inner experience.
In this case, we should note the importance of explicit beliefs in the mediation of experience.

People who are consciously involved in believing, expecting, and desiring certain experiences, thus, may also be more prone to achieving these experiences.

This can only occur when expectations are validated by the broader, and more implicit comfort of expecting that other people have similar expectations.

Anthropologists have long documented incidences of trance, dissociation, spirit possessions, and other anomalous experiences that occur in ritual, often spiritual contexts in the absence of trauma and pathology.

In such cases, such as Candomblé spirit possession in Brazil or Madagascar, these experiences are understood as normal and desirable.

Tanya Luhrmann, whose work on voices across cultures we reviewed earlier, also conducted fascinating long-term anthropological and psychological investigations of inner dimensions of prayer among Pentecostal Christians.

Luhrmann’s work demonstrated that, in a process not unlike Tulpamancy, the hard work of prayer could eventually lead to voice-hearing experiences among believers.

She initially hypothesized that learning to hear the voice of God may require a proclivity for absorption.
Her studies showed that those among her informants who reported the most vivid mental imagery, greater focus, and more intense spiritual experiences scored higher on the Tellegen Absorption Scale (TAS).

Beyond the importance of proclivities, however, a key finding of Luhrmann’s research was that absorption could be trained and improved in practice. Luhrmann’s work elegantly showed that spiritual and other unusual sensory experiences can become extraordinarily vivid as a result of attentional learning, particularly when they are sought after and rewarded in a community of people with similar beliefs.

In my own work, I also found that Tulpamancers scored higher than average on the Tellegen Absorption Scale.
Whether this reflects individual proclivities and personal types that are more likely to be interested in Tulpamancy than others is a difficult question.

My research, like Luhrmann’s, suggests that absorption-proneness can improve with practice, and that culture is an important factor in shaping the desirability and rewarding quality of unusual sensory experiences.

To make authoritative claims on Tulpamancy as an absorption-training practice, however, would require training non-Tulpamancers in the art of conjuring voices with longitudinal follow up of high and low absorption trait control groups.

Tulpamancy in popular culture: secular mysticism as resistance

Tulpamancers, as we have seen, are able to achieve highly individualized and unusual experiences that are, however, very similar in terms of their phenomenology.

It is precisely because Tulpamancy has become organized as a formalized culture (that is, a group of people united by shared expectations about the possibility and desirability of certain kinds of beings and states of affairs) that Tulpa experiences are at once possible, successful, and feel so positive to Tulpamancers.

The “fringe” dimension of Tulpamancy, on the one hand, helps foster solidarity among members and increases the experiential rewards of having achieved such hard-to-reach, highly arousing experiences.

The biases of dominant Euro-American culture on unusual experiences, and mental experiences in particular, however, also create a difficult dynamic for Tulpamancers, who are often reluctant to “come out”, even to their closest friends, relatives, and acquaintances.

As news of the culture spreads online, Tulpamancers are likely to keep enduring mockery, ostracizing, and pathologization.
In this sense, the experience of such a fringe group is not unlike that of sufis, early christians, kabbalists, sadhus, and mystics of all kinds who were at once feared, revered, and oppressed in mass societies that demand rigid conformity.

Such “mystics” threaten the very core of what most people accept as real and possible at the deepest and widest levels—their lives make us uncomfortable, because they point to the tragic limits of our imagination and the shallowness of our everyday experiences.

In the globalizing, internet-mediated world of 2016, Tulpamancers have to incur the perverse consequences of a culture in which difference is valorized in principle, but is mostly policed and punished in practice.

Our culture, paradoxically, nominally values individuality, but aggressively imposes a highly standardized framework on behaviour, which can be measured, catalogued, pathologized and punished with terrible precision.

Contemporary Euro- American culture may be the most aggressive of such frameworks ever implicitly ingrained, because it extends far and deep into other people’s thoughts and sensory experiences.

The extent to which other people’s mental lives are considered “transparent” (and hence knowable) or “opaque” (and unknowable) is another important difference found across cultures.

In contemporary Euro- America, in addition to thinking of ourselves in increasingly neurochemical terms, we think and worry excessively about what other people feel and think, and we have slowly incorporated a set of simplified medicalized assumptions and worries about how “normal”, healthy, sick and dangerous other people’s thoughts are.

Add to this a moral panic about a simplified catalogue of psychopathology, an obsession with self-authorship, and pervasive marketing from pharmacological industries, and the system of domination is almost total, because people will police their own selves before policing others.

Any private mental experience that departs from this sanitized norm will tend to be self-interpreted as scary and as the potential marker of mental illness. Given this set of problems, it is important to recognize Tulpamancy as a courageous, creative, and mystical backlash to the covert conservatism of our culture.

To sum up, Tulpamancy presents us with a fascinating case study for the study of the embodied and social nature of consciousness and cognition, and the emergence of new forms of culture and subjectivity.

It also offers an important paradigm for revising our simplistic and limiting understanding of mental illness on the one hand, and mental life and personhood on the other.
 
Last edited:


aapoltergeist.jpeg


Poltergeists are one example of passively created Thoughtform Entities.
Poltergeists take form in response to the energies within a person,
and very commonly in response to the raging hormones of pubescent teenage girls.

Poltergeist phenomena presents as separate from us but actually originates within us;
as soon as we balance and gain control, the phenomena dissipates.
That is, unless the poltergeist is given the opportunity to evolve into a distinct and intelligent entity.

~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~

Tibetan Buddhism offers the concept of "tulpa" as an apparition or object created by the power of the mind.
Walter Evans-Wentz used the term "thoughtform" in his translation of the Tibetan Book of the Dead.

French explorer and Theosophist Alexandra David-Néel wrote of tulpas as
"magic formations generated by a powerful concentration of thought."[4]

She further wrote that:

"The power of producing magic formations, tulkus or less lasting and materialized tulpas, does not,
however, belong exclusively to such mystic exalted beings [Bodhisattvas].

Any human, divine or demoniac being may be possessed of it.
The only difference comes from the degree of power,
and this depends on the strength of the concentration and the quality of the mind itself.”[5]


 
Last edited: